What blame can tell us about autism

Neuroscientists find evidence that autistic patients have trouble understanding other people’s intentions.


In the mid-1980s, a team of autism researchers theorized that one of the major features of autism is an inability to infer the thoughts of other people. This skill, known as theory of mind, comes naturally to most people — we are constantly evaluating other people’s mental states and trying to determine what they know, what they want and why they are happy or sad, angry or scared.

Though there is much anecdotal evidence that this skill is impaired in autistic people, it has been difficult to show it experimentally in adults. Now, a study from MIT neuroscientists reveals that high-functioning autistic adults appear to have trouble using theory of mind to make moral judgments in certain situations.

Specifically, the researchers found that autistic adults were more likely than non-autistic subjects to blame someone for accidentally causing harm to another person. This shows that their judgments rely more on the outcome of the incident than on an understanding of the person’s intentions, says Liane Young, an MIT postdoctoral associate and one of the lead authors of the study, which appears in the Jan. 31 online edition of the Proceedings of the National Academy of Sciences.

For example, in one scenario, “Janet” and a friend are kayaking in a part of the ocean with many jellyfish. The friend asks Janet if she should go for a swim. Janet has just read that the jellyfish in the area are harmless, and tells her friend to go for a swim. The friend is stung by a jellyfish and dies.

In this scenario, the researchers found that people with autism are more likely than non-autistic people to blame Janet for her friend’s death, even though she believed the jellyfish were harmless.

Young notes that such scenarios tend to elicit a broad range of responses even among non-autistic people. “There’s no normative truth as to whether accidents should be forgiven. The pattern with autistic patients is that they are at one end of the spectrum,” she says. Young’s co-lead author on the paper is former MIT postdoctoral associate Joseph Moran, now at Harvard.

Understanding others

Most children develop theory-of-mind ability around age 4 or 5, which can be demonstrated experimentally with “false-belief” tests. In the classic example, a child is shown two dolls, “Sally” and “Anne.” The experimenter puts on a skit in which Sally puts a marble in a basket and then leaves the scene. While Sally is away, Anne moves the marble from the basket to a box. The experimenter asks the child where Sally will look for the marble when she returns. Giving the correct answer — that Sally will look in the basket — requires an understanding that others have beliefs that may differ from our own knowledge of the world, and from reality.

Previous studies have shown that autistic children develop this ability later than non-autistic children, if ever, depending on the severity of the autism, says the study's senior author, John Gabrieli, the Grover Herman Professor of Health Sciences and Technology.

“High-functioning” autistic people — for example, those with a milder form of autism such as Asperger’s syndrome — often develop compensatory mechanisms to deal with their difficulties in understanding other people’s thoughts. The details of these mechanisms are unknown, says Young, but they allow autistic people to function in society and to pass simple experimental tests such as determining whether someone has committed a societal “faux pas.”

However, the scenarios used in the new MIT study were constructed so that there is no easy way to compensate for impaired theory of mind. The researchers tested 13 autistic adults and 13 non-autistic adults on about 50 scenarios similar to the jellyfish example.

Uta Frith, professor of psychology at the University College London’s Institute of Cognitive Neuroscience, was part of the team of scientists who published research in 1985 suggesting that children with autism have impairments of theory of mind. Frith says the new study offers strong support for that hypothesis.

“The study uses a novel test, the judgment of intentions in moral vignettes, to probe the theory. It is an excellent test because it detects poor mentalizing even in individuals with mild autism spectrum disorder who have learned to compensate for their difficulties in understanding mental states,” she says.

Building morality

In a 2010 study, Young used the same hypothetical scenarios to test the moral judgments of a group of patients with damage to the ventromedial prefrontal cortex (VMPC), a part of the prefrontal cortex (where planning, decision-making and other complex cognitive tasks occur).

Those patients understand other people’s intentions, but they lack the emotional outrage that usually occurs in cases where someone tries (but fails) to harm someone else. For example, they would more easily forgive someone who offers mushrooms he believes to be poisonous to an acquaintance, if the mushrooms turn out to be harmless.

“While autistic individuals are unable to process mental state information and understand that individuals can have innocent intentions, the issue with VMPC patients is that they could understand information but did not respond emotionally to that information,” says Young.

Putting these two pieces together could help neuroscientists come up with a more thorough picture of how the brain constructs morality. Previous studies by MIT assistant professor Rebecca Saxe (also an author of the new PNAS paper) have shown that theory of mind appears to be seated in a brain region called the right temporoparietal junction (TPJ). In ongoing studies, the researchers are studying whether autistic patients have irregular activity in the right TPJ while performing the moral judgment tasks used in the PNAS study.


Topics: Autism, Brain and cognitive sciences, Neuroscience

Comments

From what I can tell, the development of the brain can be described as the increasing ability to take on larger levels of perspectives for any individual situation. For example, at a very early age, the brain can assess things from only an internal/self perspective. Then, in humans, at about the age of 9 months, it can start to look at things from a second "person" perspective (the person being anything, not just a living thing), which is sometimes called ethno-centric thinking. And then just before age 4 in humans we can see things from a third person perspective, sometimes called world-centric thinking. And it looks like there is one final jump at around age 35 in humans, where we can start to see a more universe-centric way, with fourth person perspective. For a map of this, check out: http://sites.google.com/site/turilsstuff/home/relationshipgrowth.gif
But I would take issue with this "black and white" assessment. Autism is a complex disorder--shall we say, rather, CONDITIONS, as is dyslexia. I have known people with either "diagnosis", and must argue that persons with many with diagnoses of autism and dyslexia are not without empathy or compassion, but interpret dire consequences with perhaps a different outward expression of such emotion--even cloaking some degree of hypersensitivity, even. Such folks are without these feeling--theirs is often "presented" with a non-perfected Emily Post---or even too much so, in some cases. Furthermore, such seeming and blanketing condemnation of a population rather arbitrarily assessed with generalized characteristics might well lend to very dire and even prejudicial and inaccurate face-value assessment of true nature. One would hope that said researchers take a more detailed and objective scientific assessment of social and now medical afflictions. The spectrum assessment here is far too narrow--and, frankly, stupid. Let's look at Kathleen Seidel types and others who vigorous defend that autism is not a disease. They have some points to the contrary of this rather base and limited perspective that those with autism and dyslexia are incapable of feeling... one might argue that to some extent, folks going about their lives are, too, very focused to a point of deflective indifference re: "important" and feeling concerns. Are these matters of care and feeling "neuro-connected"? Alas, the larger issues of nature v nurture, and from these sorts of studies, it might appear that only "nature", ie, genetics and diagnoses (as loose as such can be, either) are being addressed, ie, half, or whatever combination, of what's relevant.....the other part is human interpretation..which is "social science" and far more loose and unattainable.....in interpretation and feeling.
I do like the theories shown here, and from personal experience (I have the same "high functioning" autism as mentioned. Aspergers, too, 25 years old.) it gives me insight on how I understand the world. I know that in my particular case I had absolutely no care at all for anyone outside of my own family. I hated all others, for the longest time, until high-school. But I think in my case, it was based on intelligence, not on intention (I liked my teachers). Given the jellyfish situation, I might have said the box, but only because that's where she "should" have looked to find the ball. It could be a matter of misunderstanding what was wanted. What was wanted (Where sally would look "first"), is different from what we believe was asked (Where sally will look to find the ball). It could be, also, that we simply ignore the fact of the ball entirely, and think about what we would do, but leave out the correct "basket" answer because we want to find the ball. Just my two cents.
Interesting comment, and I'd have to mostly agree. Autistic people (Like me) were never without emotion. We simply never understood how to show it. Some of us never will understand. Perhaps because we don't care to. I know that it wasn't until high school that I started to care about really learning social cues, learning "why" people do the things they do. It wasn't until I started caring about others that I started learning why others don't care. Even myself. I hate to use nature vs. nurture here, though, because a LOT of my life was based on both. By nature, I'm similar to a computer. Learning feelings, emotion, everything, but in an abstract way, not actually feeling it. But through nurture, through concrete feelings like pain, and suffering, I was finally able to understand what love felt like. And that is when I started to really enjoy life. Autistic people are very intelligent. But we are smarter on the things that don't change. We (generally) like tradition.
Back to the top